早稲田法 2015 II
フレーズ訳・解説はこちら
II Read the passage and answer the questions below.
II Read the passage and answer the questions below.
① Throughout history, the lack of knowledge
about what is going on in the minds of infants and animals has generated
guesswork, projection, and imagination. But in the past twenty years, ingenious
laboratory experiments and discoveries in evolutionary biology have been
opening windows of understanding that had once been thought permanently
shuttered. Contemporary developmental research is changing our understanding of
the interaction between nature and nurture.
② Paul Bloom, a developmental psychologist
at Yale University, argues that Thomas Jefferson, the American philosopher and
president, was right when he wrote: "The moral sense, or conscience, is as
much a part of man as his leg or arm. It is given to all human beings in a
stronger or weaker degree." Some aspects of morality, says Bloom,
"come naturally to us," including empathy and compassion, an ability
to distinguish kind from cruel actions, a rudimentary sense of fairness, and a
rudimentary sense of justice. "Rudimentary" is critical, of course.
No one disputes that culture, family, and society work on those rudiments to
shape, enhance, or extinguish those qualities.
③ Still, how would one measure "the
moral sense" in a baby? Here's an example from Bloom that illustrates the
method and the results:
The
one-year-old decided to take justice into his own hands. He had just watched a
puppet show with three characters. The puppet in the middle rolled a ball to
the puppet on the right, who passed it right back to him. It then rolled the
ball to the puppet on the left, who ran away with it. At the end of the show,
the "nice" puppet and the "naughty" puppet were brought
down from the stage and set before the boy. A treat was placed in front of each
of them, and the boy was invited to take one of the treats away. As predicted,
and like most toddlers in this experiment, he took it from the "naughty"
one―the one who ran away with the ball. But this wasn't enough. The boy then
leaned over and smacked the puppet on the head.
④ Bloom explains that by "moral"
he means an inherent sense of right and wrong that is not learned, not taught
in school or by religion, but which is the product of biological evolution. At
its base, he argues, morality reflects our gut feelings that certain acts are
just plain wrong―cruel, unkind, unfair, violations of human dignity. Any theory
of moral psychology has to explain how these intuitions work and where they
come from.
⑤ Although the notion of the "rational
man" has long predicted that people seek to maximize their own gains, the
fact is that the "fairness bias" often trumps selfishness. "It's
not fair!" a small child will cry, and is usually right. Monkeys offered a
treat they would normally enjoy may throw it on the ground in disgust if they see
a neighboring monkey getting a better treat, and dogs, chimpanzees, and young children
show the same signs of being bothered if they get a smaller reward than someone
else. Or consider a two-person psychological experiment called the Ultimatum
Game, in which your partner gets a sum of money and must decide how much to
share with you. You can choose to accept your partner's offer, in which case
you both get to keep your respective portions, or you can reject the offer, in
which case neither of you gets a cent. How low an offer would you accept? It
makes "rational" sense to accept any amount at all, no matter how
insignificant, because then at least you will get something. But that is not
how people respond in this game. If the offer is too low, they are likely to
reject it. For players in industrial societies, offers below 20 or 30 percent
are commonly rejected, even when the amounts they would receive are large. For
players in other societies, the percentages may be higher or lower, but there is
always some percentage that people consider unfair and refuse to accept.
⑥ Cooperative tendencies and a desire for
fairness evolved because they were beneficial to our ancestors, ensuring
teamwork and harmony among members of a group. Unfortunately, the evolutionary price
of that harmony and helpfulness toward our own kind is hostility and prejudice
toward strangers and competition with outgroups. Currently, there is a
fashionable temptation to embrace biological and evolutionary reductionism to
explain this, to believe that "that's how we are built, for better or
worse; we can't do much about it; we are irrational creatures after all."
The missing ingredient in such an account is reason. Reason has driven not only
scientific discoveries, but also moral progress, such as awareness of the
wrongness of slavery. Moral progress is learned in individuals; kindness toward
strangers is lacking in babies and young children.
⑦An encompassing theory of our moral lives
must acknowledge its two parts. The first is the rich biological inheritance
that evolution has provided: empathy, compassion, the capacity to judge the
actions of others, and a rudimentary understanding of justice and fairness. We
are more than just babies, however; a critical part of our morality―so much of
what makes us human―emerges over the course of human history and individual
development. It is the product of our compassion, our imagination, and our magnificent
capacity for reason.
[Adapted from Carol Tavris, "Cradles
of Civilization," Times Literary Supplement (June 13, 2014).]
(1) Choose the best way to complete the
following sentences about Paragraphs ① to ⑦
1 In Paragraph ① the
writer mainly
2 In Paragraph ②
the writer mainly
3 In Paragraph ③
the writer mainly
4 In Paragraph ④
the writer mainly
5 In Paragraph. ⑤
the writer mainly
6 In Paragraph ⑥
the writer mainly
7 In Paragraph ⑦
the writer mainly
A argues that human morality is based
partly on an inherited ability to empathize with others, and partly on what we
learn over the course of time.
B casts doubt on the notion that a person
will accept any small gain, even if it is less than what another receives.
C criticizes the views of those who dispute
that culture, family, and society influence the development of human morality.
D describes a psychologist's remark about a
former American leader's belief that all human beings have at least some sense
of what is right and what is wrong.
E explains that new research enables us to
see into the minds of those who cannot explain themselves in words, such as
babies and animals.
F lists forms of entertainment, such as
violent puppet shows, that human beings once enjoyed but no longer do.
G maintains that babies and young children
have a moral sense that makes them sympathetic to strangers.
H outlines an experiment that shows that
most people will accept 20 to 30 percent of an offer to share, so long as that
offer comes from a partner from the same society.
I points out that there is nothing we can
do about our tendency to feel hostility toward strangers, because such feelings
developed through human evolution.
J provides evidence that babies as young as
twelve months old understand the concepts of "fair" and
"unfair."
K puts forward one researcher's definition
of the concept of "moral."
L states that it is possible to overcome
our suspicion of people who are different from us by the use of reason.
(2) Choose the ONE way to complete each of
these sentences that is NOT correct according to the passage.
1 Babies and young children
A have a moral sense that researchers have
recently found ways to observe.
B in the experiment tended to take the
treat from the puppet they felt had behaved unfairly.
C lack feelings of compassion for those
they do not know.
D lack knowledge, imagination, and the
ability to generate guesswork.
E were once thought to have minds to which
researchers could not gain access.
2 In the Ultimatum Game
A one player can accept the offer of the
other player to share a set amount of money, and in return both players can
keep their respective portions.
B one player can reject the offer of the
other player to share a set amount of money, and in return neither player
receives anything.
C one player must decide how much of a set
amount of money to share with the other player.
D players from the industrialized world are
reluctant to accept less than a third to a fifth of a set amount of money.
E players from the non-industrialized world
usually agree to accept any amount of money, so that both players can keep
their respective portions.
3 Moral sense
A is something that develops in different
directions, depending on the culture we are born into and our family life.
B is something that enables us to tell
right from wrong, fair from unfair, and compassionate from cruel.
C is something that evolved because it
helped us to recognize members of our own group and behave with hostility to
outsiders.
D is something that we are born with and
exists prior to whatever influences we receive from religion and/or education.
E is something that we are born with and
yet is also subject to reason, which accounts for our ability to make moral
progress.
(3) Choose the best way to complete each of
these sentences, which refer to the underlined words in the passage.
1 Here "rudimentary" means
A acceptable. B basic. C easy. D
satisfactory. E systematic.
2 Here "trumps" means
A beats. B invents. C prejudices. D
simplifies. E threatens.
3 Here "embrace" means
A avoid. B conceal. C hug. D include. E
oppose.
(4) Find the vowel with the strongest
stress in each of these words, as used in the passage. Choose the ONE which is
pronounced DIFFERENTLY in each group of five.
1 A compassion B empathy C fashionable D
interaction E naturally
2 A dignity B intuition C predicted D
religion E rudiments
3 A embrace B irrational C slavery D
strangers E ultimatum










コメント
コメントを投稿