早稲田法 2015 II フレーズ訳・解説


原文はこちら

 
Throughout history, 
the lack of knowledge 
about what is going on 
     in the minds of infants and animals 
has generated guesswork, projection, and imagination. 

what is going on「何が起きているか」

But in the past twenty years, 
ingenious laboratory experiments and discoveries 
in evolutionary biology 
have been opening windows 
of understanding 
that had once been thought 
 permanently shuttered

in evolutionary biology → in「~の分野で」

Contemporary developmental research 
is changing our understanding 
of the interaction between nature and nurture.

 
Paul Bloom
, a developmental psychologist at Yale University, 
argues that 
Thomas Jefferson
, the American philosopher and president, 
was right 
when he wrote: 

"The moral sense, or conscience, 
is as much a part of man 
as his leg or arm. 
It is given to all human beings 
in a stronger or weaker degree." 

argue that 「~と言う、主張する」
as much A as B Bと同様にAである」

Some aspects of morality
, says Bloom, 
"come naturally to us," 
including empathy and compassion, 
an ability to distinguish kind from cruel actions, 
a rudimentary sense of fairness, 
and a rudimentary sense of justice. 

"Rudimentary" is critical, of course. 

No one disputes that 
culture, family, and society 
work on those rudiments 
to shape, enhance, or extinguish those qualities.

 
Still, 
how would one measure "the moral sense" 
in a baby? 

Here's an example from Bloom 
that illustrates the method and the results:

The one-year-old 
decided to take justice into his own hands. 

He had just watched 
a puppet show with three characters. 

The puppet in the middle 
rolled a ball 
to the puppet on the right
, who passed it right back to him. 

who = the puppet on the right
him = the puppet in the middle

It then rolled the ball 
to the puppet on the left
, who ran away with it. 

It then → It = the puppet in the middle
who = the puppet on the left
with it → it = the ball

At the end of the show, 
the "nice" puppet and the "naughty" puppet 
were brought down from the stage 
and set before the boy. 

A treat was placed 
in front of each of them, 
and the boy was invited 
to take one of the treats away. 

take O awayOを取り上げる」

As predicted, 
and like most toddlers in this experiment, 
he took it from the "naughty" one
―the one who ran away with the ball. 

But this wasn't enough. 

The boy then leaned over 
and smacked the puppet on the head.

 
Bloom explains that 
by "moral" 
he means 
an inherent sense of right and wrong 
that is not learned, 
 not taught in school 
 or by religion
but which is the product of biological evolution.

Bloomは説明する
"moral"という言葉で
彼は意味する
遺伝する感覚/善悪の
〔それは学ばれない
 教えられない/学校で
 また宗教によっても〕
そうではなく〔産物である/生物学的進化の〕


not A but BAでなくてB


At its base, 
he argues, 
morality reflects our gut feelings 
that certain acts are just plain wrong
 ―cruel, unkind, unfair, violations of human dignity

at its base「その根底で」
gut feeling「直感」→gut「内臓」

Any theory of moral psychology 
has to explain 
how these intuitions work 
and where they come from.

 
Although 
 the notion of the "rational man" 
 has long predicted that 
    people seek to maximize their own gains, 
the fact is that 
the "fairness bias" often trumps selfishness. 

trump「~に勝る」

"It's not fair!" 
a small child will cry, 
and is usually right. 

Monkeys 
offered a treat 
they would normally enjoy 
may throw it on the ground 
in disgust 
if they see a neighboring monkey 
getting a better treat, 
and dogs, chimpanzees, and young children 
show the same signs of being bothered 
if they get a smaller reward 
than someone else. 

offered a treat「ご褒美を差し出された」
see O doingOが~しているのを見る」

Or consider a two-person psychological experiment 
called the Ultimatum Game
in which your partner gets a sum of money 
 and must decide how much to share with you

in which = in the experiment

You can choose to accept your partner's offer, 
in which case 
you both get to keep your respective portions, 
or you can reject the offer, 
in which case 
neither of you gets a cent. 

in which case「その場合は」
neither of ......のどちらも~ない」

How low an offer would you accept?

It makes "rational" sense
to accept any amount at all,
no matter how insignificant,
because then
at least you will get something.

It = to accept ...
any amount at all 「どんな量でも」

But that is not
how people respond in this game.

If the offer is too low,
they are likely to reject it.

For players in industrial societies,
offers below 20 or 30 percent
are commonly rejected,
even when the amounts they would receive
are large.

For players in other societies,
the percentages may be higher or lower,
but there is always some percentage
that people consider unfair and refuse to accept.

 
Cooperative tendencies and a desire for fairness
evolved
because they were beneficial to our ancestors,
ensuring teamwork and harmony
among members of a group.

Unfortunately,
the evolutionary price of that harmony
and helpfulness toward our own kind
is hostility and prejudice toward strangers
and competition with outgroups.

price 「代償」
helpfulness toward 「~に役立とうとすること」

Currently,
there is a fashionable temptation
to embrace biological and evolutionary reductionism
to explain this,
to believe that
"that's how we are built, for better or worse;
we can't do much about it;
we are irrational creatures after all."

The missing ingredient
in such an account
is reason.

Reason has driven
not only scientific discoveries,
but also moral progress,
such as awareness of the wrongness of slavery.

drive 「~を促進する」

Moral progress is learned in individuals;
kindness toward strangers
is lacking in babies and young children.


An encompassing theory of our moral lives
must acknowledge its two parts.

The first is the rich biological inheritance
that evolution has provided:
empathy, compassion,
the capacity to judge the actions of others,
and a rudimentary understanding
of justice and fairness.

We are more than just babies, however;
a critical part of our morality
―so much of what makes us human―
emerges over the course of human history
and individual development.

so much of 「~の大部分」
what makes us human 「私たちを人間にしているもの人間らしさ」

It is the product
of our compassion,
our imagination,
and our magnificent capacity for reason.



(1)

A
argues that
human morality is based
partly on an inherited ability
to empathize with others,
and partly on what we learn over the course of time.


be based on 「~に基づいている」

B
casts doubt on the notion that
a person will accept any small gain,
even if it is less than what another receives.


C
criticizes the views
of those who dispute that
culture, family, and society
influence the development of human morality.


D
describes a psychologist's remark
about a former American leader's belief that
all human beings have at least some sense
of what is right and what is wrong.


E
explains that
new research enables us
to see into the minds
of those who cannot explain themselves in words,
such as babies and animals.


F
lists forms of entertainment,
such as violent puppet shows,
that human beings once enjoyed but no longer do.


once 「かつて」
no longer 「もはや~ない」

G
maintains that babies and young children
have a moral sense
that makes them sympathetic to strangers.


H
outlines an experiment
that shows that
most people will accept
20 to 30 percent of an offer to share,
so long as
that offer comes from a partner
from the same society.


so long as 「~限り」

I
points out that
there is nothing we can do
about our tendency
to feel hostility toward strangers,
because such feelings developed
through human evolution.


J
provides evidence that
babies
as young as twelve months old
understand the concepts of "fair" and "unfair."


K
puts forward one researcher's definition
of the concept of "moral."


put forward 「提案する、提示する」

L
states that
it is possible
to overcome our suspicion of people
who are different from us
by the use of reason.


state 「述べる」



コメント

このブログの人気の投稿

The Secret Garden (Oxford Bookworms Level 3)

Global Issues (Oxford Bookworms Level 3)

早稲田商2017 II フレーズ訳