早稲田法 2015 II フレーズ訳・解説
原文はこちら
①
Throughout
history,
the
lack of knowledge
about
what is going on
in the minds of infants and animals
has
generated guesswork, projection, and imagination.
※what is going on「何が起きているか」
But in
the past twenty years,
ingenious
laboratory experiments and discoveries
in
evolutionary biology
have
been opening windows
of
understanding
〔that had once been thought
permanently shuttered〕.
※in evolutionary biology → in「~の分野で」
Contemporary
developmental research
is
changing our understanding
of the
interaction between nature and nurture.
②
Paul
Bloom
, a
developmental psychologist at Yale University,
argues
that
Thomas
Jefferson
, the
American philosopher and president,
was
right
when
he wrote:
"The
moral sense, or conscience,
is as
much a part of man
as his
leg or arm.
It is
given to all human beings
in a
stronger or weaker degree."
※argue that 「~と言う、主張する」
※as much A as B 「Bと同様にAである」
Some
aspects of morality
, says
Bloom,
"come
naturally to us,"
including
empathy and compassion,
an
ability to distinguish kind from cruel actions,
a
rudimentary sense of fairness,
and a
rudimentary sense of justice.
"Rudimentary"
is critical, of course.
No one
disputes that
culture,
family, and society
work
on those rudiments
to
shape, enhance, or extinguish those qualities.
③
Still,
how
would one measure "the moral sense"
in a
baby?
Here's
an example from Bloom
〔that illustrates the method and the results〕:
The
one-year-old
decided
to take justice into his own hands.
He had
just watched
a
puppet show with three characters.
The
puppet in the middle
rolled
a ball
to the
puppet on the right
, who
passed it right back to him.
※who = the puppet on the right
※him = the puppet in the middle
It
then rolled the ball
to the
puppet on the left
, who
ran away with it.
※It then → It = the puppet in the middle
※who = the puppet on the left
※with it → it = the ball
At the
end of the show,
the
"nice" puppet and the "naughty" puppet
were
brought down from the stage
and
set before the boy.
A
treat was placed
in
front of each of them,
and
the boy was invited
to
take one of the treats away.
※take O away「Oを取り上げる」
As
predicted,
and
like most toddlers in this experiment,
he
took it from the "naughty" one
―the
one who ran away with the ball.
But
this wasn't enough.
The
boy then leaned over
and
smacked the puppet on the head.
④
Bloom
explains that
by
"moral"
he
means
an
inherent sense of right and wrong
〔that is not learned,
not taught in school
or by religion〕,
but 〔which is the product of biological evolution〕.
Bloomは説明する
"moral"という言葉で
彼は意味する
遺伝する感覚/善悪の
〔それは学ばれない
教えられない/学校で
また宗教によっても〕
そうではなく〔産物である/生物学的進化の〕
※not A but B「AでなくてB」
At its
base,
he
argues,
morality
reflects our gut feelings
〔that certain acts are just plain wrong
―cruel, unkind, unfair, violations of human dignity〕.
※at its base「その根底で」
※gut feeling「直感」→gut「内臓」
Any
theory of moral psychology
has to
explain
how
these intuitions work
and
where they come from.
⑤
Although
the notion of the "rational man"
has long predicted that
people seek to maximize their own gains,
the fact
is that
the
"fairness bias" often trumps selfishness.
※trump「~に勝る」
"It's
not fair!"
a
small child will cry,
and is
usually right.
Monkeys
〔offered a treat〕
〔they would normally enjoy〕
may
throw it on the ground
in
disgust
if
they see a neighboring monkey
getting
a better treat,
and
dogs, chimpanzees, and young children
show
the same signs of being bothered
if
they get a smaller reward
than
someone else.
※offered a treat「ご褒美を差し出された」
※see O doing「Oが~しているのを見る」
Or consider
a two-person psychological experiment
〔called the Ultimatum Game〕,
〔in which your partner gets a sum of money
and must decide how much to share with you〕.
※in which = in the experiment
You
can choose to accept your partner's offer,
in
which case
you
both get to keep your respective portions,
or you
can reject the offer,
in
which case
neither
of you gets a cent.
※in which case「その場合は」
※neither of ...「...のどちらも~ない」
How
low an offer would you accept?
It
makes "rational" sense
to
accept any amount at all,
no
matter how insignificant,
because
then
at
least you will get something.
※It = to accept ...
※any amount at all 「どんな量でも」
But
that is not
how
people respond in this game.
If the
offer is too low,
they
are likely to reject it.
For
players in industrial societies,
offers
below 20 or 30 percent
are
commonly rejected,
even
when the amounts 〔they would receive〕
are
large.
For
players in other societies,
the
percentages may be higher or lower,
but
there is always some percentage
〔that people consider unfair and refuse to accept〕.
⑥
Cooperative
tendencies and a desire for fairness
evolved
because
they were beneficial to our ancestors,
ensuring
teamwork and harmony
among
members of a group.
Unfortunately,
the
evolutionary price of that harmony
and
helpfulness toward our own kind
is
hostility and prejudice toward strangers
and
competition with outgroups.
※price 「代償」
※helpfulness toward 「~に役立とうとすること」
Currently,
there
is a fashionable temptation
to
embrace biological and evolutionary reductionism
to
explain this,
to
believe that
"that's
how we are built, for better or worse;
we
can't do much about it;
we are
irrational creatures after all."
The
missing ingredient
in
such an account
is
reason.
Reason
has driven
not
only scientific discoveries,
but
also moral progress,
such
as awareness of the wrongness of slavery.
※drive 「~を促進する」
Moral
progress is learned in individuals;
kindness
toward strangers
is
lacking in babies and young children.
⑦
An
encompassing theory of our moral lives
must
acknowledge its two parts.
The
first is the rich biological inheritance
〔that evolution has provided〕:
empathy,
compassion,
the
capacity to judge the actions of others,
and a
rudimentary understanding
of
justice and fairness.
We are
more than just babies, however;
a
critical part of our morality
―so
much of what makes us human―
emerges
over the course of human history
and
individual development.
※so much of 「~の大部分」
※what makes us human 「私たちを人間にしているもの→人間らしさ」
It is
the product
of our
compassion,
our
imagination,
and
our magnificent capacity for reason.
(1)
A
argues
that
human
morality is based
partly
on an inherited ability
to
empathize with others,
and
partly on what we learn over the course of time.
※be based on 「~に基づいている」
B
casts
doubt on the notion that
a
person will accept any small gain,
even
if it is less than what another receives.
C
criticizes
the views
of
those who dispute that
culture,
family, and society
influence
the development of human morality.
D
describes
a psychologist's remark
about
a former American leader's belief that
all
human beings have at least some sense
of
what is right and what is wrong.
E
explains
that
new
research enables us
to see
into the minds
of
those who cannot explain themselves in words,
such
as babies and animals.
F
lists
forms of entertainment,
such
as violent puppet shows,
that
human beings once enjoyed but no longer do.
※once 「かつて」
※no longer 「もはや~ない」
G
maintains
that babies and young children
have a
moral sense
〔that makes them sympathetic to strangers〕.
H
outlines
an experiment
〔that shows that
most people will accept
20 to 30 percent of an offer to share,
so long as
that offer comes from a partner
from the same society〕.
※so long as 「~限り」
I
points
out that
there
is nothing we can do
about
our tendency
to
feel hostility toward strangers,
because
such feelings developed
through
human evolution.
J
provides
evidence that
babies
as
young as twelve months old
understand
the concepts of "fair" and "unfair."
K
puts
forward one researcher's definition
of the
concept of "moral."
※put forward 「提案する、提示する」
L
states
that
it is
possible
to
overcome our suspicion of people
〔who are different from us〕
by the
use of reason.
※state 「述べる」
コメント
コメントを投稿